Successful honey-hunters know how to communicate with wild birds

January 23, 2024

The experiments showed that honeyguides in the Kidero Hills, Tanzania are over three times more likely to cooperate with people giving the local Hadza whistle, than people giving the ‘foreign’ Yao trill and grunt. And the honeyguides in the Niassa Special Reserve, Mozambique are almost twice as likely to cooperate in response to the local Yao trill and grunt, than the ‘foreign’ Hadza whistle. The phenomenon seems to be self-reinforcing: honeyguides learn to recognise that a specific call indicates a good honey-hunting partner, and humans are more successful in attracting the birds if they use this call. The researchers compare this to different human languages, in which the sounds of words are arbitrary, but everyone has agreed on their meaning. Spottiswoode added: “Just as humans across the world communicate using a range of different local languages, people across Africa communicate with honeyguide birds using a range of different local sounds.”Like language, these culturally determined calls convey an underlying meaning – signalling a desire to partner with the bird to find honey.

The experiments showed that honeyguides in the Kidero Hills, Tanzania are over three times more likely to cooperate with people giving the local Hadza whistle, than people giving the ‘foreign’ Yao trill and grunt. And the honeyguides in the Niassa Special Reserve, Mozambique are almost twice as likely to cooperate in response to the local Yao trill and grunt, than the ‘foreign’ Hadza whistle.

The phenomenon seems to be self-reinforcing: honeyguides learn to recognise that a specific call indicates a good honey-hunting partner, and humans are more successful in attracting the birds if they use this call.

People who use a different call are less likely to attract a bird to guide them to the honey – so it’s in their interests to stick to the sounds used locally.

“Once these local cultural traditions are established, it pays for everyone – birds and humans – to conform to them, even if the sounds themselves are arbitrary,” said joint lead author Dr Brian Wood, an anthropologist at the University of California, Los Angeles.

The researchers compare this to different human languages, in which the sounds of words are arbitrary, but everyone has agreed on their meaning.

Spottiswoode added: “Just as humans across the world communicate using a range of different local languages, people across Africa communicate with honeyguide birds using a range of different local sounds.”

Like language, these culturally determined calls convey an underlying meaning – signalling a desire to partner with the bird to find honey.

The source of this news is from University of Cambridge

Popular in Research

1

Jul 7, 2024

Scientists use generative AI to answer complex questions in physics

2

Jul 7, 2024

First language song book a hit in the APY Lands

3

Jul 7, 2024

Navigating longevity with industry leaders at MIT AgeLab PLAN Forum

4

Jul 7, 2024

Dismissed and discharged: health systems still failing people with poor mental health

5

Jul 7, 2024

Elaine Liu: Charging ahead

Biden’s ABC Interview Was a Necessary Appointment With the Public — and a Botched One

Jul 7, 2024

No “Serious Condition”: Watch Biden Tell George Stephanopoulos Of Debate Debacle In First Clip From ABC Interview

Jul 6, 2024

American Air, Gate Gourmet Face Pressure on Contracts to Avoid Strikes

Jul 7, 2024

MSN

Jul 7, 2024

NYU Dentistry Names Implant Dentistry Fellowship in Recognition of Major Gift from Alumni Noel Liu and Nazish Jafri

Jul 7, 2024

Biden Aides Provided Questions in Advance for His Radio Interviews

Jul 7, 2024